Elsevier

Intelligence

Volume 53, November–December 2015, Pages 118-137
Intelligence

Intelligence and school grades: A meta-analysis

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2015.09.002Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Psychometric meta-analysis on the relation between intelligence and school grades.

  • Intelligence is a very strong predictor of school grades.

  • The type of intelligence test moderates the intelligence–school grades relation.

  • School-related factors (i.e., school subject, grade level) affect the relationship.

  • The relevance of intelligence for school grades has changed over time.

Abstract

Intelligence is considered as the strongest predictor of scholastic achievement. Research as well as educational policy and the society as a whole are deeply interested in its role as a prerequisite for scholastic success. The present study investigated the population correlation between standardized intelligence tests and school grades employing psychometric meta-analysis (Hunter & Schmidt, 2004). The analyses involved 240 independent samples with 105,185 participants overall. After correcting for sampling error, error of measurement, and range restriction in the independent variable, we found a population correlation of ρ = .54. Moderator analyses pointed to a variation of the relationship between g and school grades depending on different school subject domains, grade levels, the type of intelligence test used in the primary study, as well as the year of publication, whereas gender had no effect on the magnitude of the relationship.

Introduction

Intelligence is the strongest predictor of academic achievement with correlations ranging from .30 to .70 (e.g., Chamorro-Premuzic and Furnham, 2005, Colom and Flores-Mendoza, 2007, Deary et al., 2007, Gottfredson, 2002, Gustafsson and Undheim, 1996, Jensen, 1998, Kuncel et al., 2004, Kyttälä and Lehto, 2008, Laidra et al., 2007, Lemos et al., 2014, Neisser et al., 1996, Primi et al., 2010, Rosander et al., 2011, Taub et al., 2008). Well known and much-quoted reviews (e.g., Gottfredson, 2002, Gustafsson and Undheim, 1996, Neisser et al., 1996, Sternberg et al., 2001) refer to a mean correlation of .5, but none of them cites a study in which this was investigated. This is not surprising, since there was (and is) no current comprehensive meta-analytic examination of the association between g and scholastic achievement. Previous meta-analyses (see the following section) present data assessed before 1983, focus only on natural sciences and specific countries and do not correct for artifacts which might lower the correlation (i.e., unreliability, range restriction). Moreover, scholastic achievement is measured by achievement tests instead of school grades as a direct measure of scholastic success. However, school grades are crucial for accessing further scholastic and occupational qualification, and therefore, have an enormous influence on an individual's life (Sauer, 2006, Tent, 2006). With this study we try to close this research gap by integrating the extensive body of knowledge concerning the correlation between g and scholastic achievement measured by school grades. The main goals of the study were the following: (1) Consideration of all available studies presented in the international literature, (2) presentation of the mean correlation only weighted by sample size as well as the true score correlation corrected for unreliability and range restriction, (3) consideration of moderator variables which might influence the correlation.

This study dealt with the correlation of cognitive ability assessed by different standardized intelligence tests and school achievement in natural sciences in grade levels 6 to 12. Included were 34 studies between 1963 and 1978 yielding 62 correlations (total N not reported). The correlations were integrated by computing the mean correlation and corresponding standard deviations. For the complete sample, a mean correlation of M(r) = .48 with a standard deviation of SD(r) = .15 was found. Furthermore, the mean correlations on different levels of a set of potential moderator variables were computed and compared using a t-test. Among several tested moderator variables, only the reliability of the outcome measure [r < .80: M(r) = .42 vs. r  .80: M(r) = .55; p = .01] had a significant influence on the strength of the relationship between cognitive ability and school achievement.

The meta-analysis of Fleming and Malone (1983) analyzed correlations of different student variables (among others general ability, verbal and mathematical ability) and scholastic achievement in natural sciences. It was based on 42 correlation coefficients (number of studies and total N not reported) between 1960 and 1981. Grade levels ranged from kindergarten to grade level 12. Intelligence was assessed by verbal and mathematical scholastic aptitude tests (SAT), scholastic achievement by standardized tests. The meta-analysis was based on the strategy of Glass, McGaw, and Smith (1981). For all studies, the mean true effect was ρ = .43 with a standard deviation of σρ = .22. Analyses of moderator variables revealed a partially moderating effect of different grade levels (Elementary School: ρ = .25; σρ = .20; Middle School: ρ = .59; σρ = .12; High School: ρ = .47; σρ = .36).

This meta-analysis integrated correlations between affect, cognitive ability and scholastic achievement in natural sciences. Since a central goal of this study was to analyze gender effects, only studies reporting gender-specific correlations were considered. For cognitive ability, 60 coefficients between 1965 and 1983 were found (number of studies and total N not reported), which were based exclusively on anglophone individuals. Grade levels ranged from elementary school to high school. Cognitive ability was assessed by standardized intelligence tests, scholastic ability by standardized and unstandardized tests. The authors employed the meta-analytic strategy of Glass (1977). For all studies, the mean true effect was ρ = .34 (σρ not reported), with no significant effect for gender.

Ranging between ρ = .34 and .48, the mean correlation between cognitive abilities and scholastic achievement investigated in previous meta-analyses was slightly lower than generally assumed in the literature (e.g., Neisser et al., 1996). A wide range of possible moderator variables was analyzed, with significant effects only for grade level (Fleming & Malone, 1983) and the reliability of the outcome measure (Boulanger, 1981). As there was a strong focus on achievement in scientific school subjects, the bulk of primary studies addressing the impact of cognitive abilities for school achievement in other subject domains was not considered, nor were potential differences in the mean correlation between g and school grades across these subject domains analyzed.

Our goal was to identify the empirical estimate of the population correlation between g and scholastic success. We argue that school grades have a much stronger effect on an individual's subsequent school and occupational career than alternative measures of school achievement (e.g., teacher ratings, school achievement tests). Therefore we focus on scholastic success in a strict sense which means that we use school grades as a criterion exclusively.

The current study aims at investigating the population correlation between g and school grades in general and without restrictions on a specific subject domain or grade level as well as the country where the data were collected and the year the study was published. Moreover, we illustrate the moderating effect of third variables on the relationship between g and school grades.

To analyze moderating effects, we formulated hypotheses about potential moderating variables. We derived our assumptions from the previous meta-analyses by Boulanger (1981), Fleming and Malone (1983), and Steinkamp and Maehr (1983) as well as from the general literature on the topic. Thus, we identified five potential moderators which are presented below (details on the coding process for moderator variables will be presented in the method section).

According to Gaedike (1974) and Sauer (2006), the performance in verbal intelligence tests is related more strongly to scholastic success than is the achievement in nonverbal ones. To test the moderating effect of the verbal or nonverbal character of intelligence tests, we built subgroups for either completely verbal or nonverbal intelligence tests as well as for such measurement instruments consisting of both verbal and nonverbal scales.

Previous meta-analyses (Boulanger, 1981, Fleming and Malone, 1983, Steinkamp and Maehr, 1983) concentrated on the mean correlation between g and school achievement in scientific school subjects. There are other school subjects beyond mathematics and science, which have not been considered in meta-analyses before. Hence we aimed at covering these and estimating the population correlation between g and school grades in a range of different school subjects. To reduce complexity and to reach a clear overview, we clustered the school subjects considered in the included primary studies into the following subgroups: Mathematics and Science (including e.g., mathematics, biology, and physics), Languages (including e.g., English, German, reading, and literature), Social Sciences (including e.g., social studies, history, and geography), Fine Art and Music, as well as Sports.

The moderating effect of grade level on the correlation between general mental ability and school grades was analyzed by Boulanger (1981), Fleming and Malone (1983), and Steinkamp and Maehr (1983). Apart from that, Brody (1992) and Jensen (1998) point out the variation of the predictive value of g for scholastic success against the background of different grade levels. Jensen (1998) refers to correlations between g and grades which decrease from elementary school (.60 to .70) throughout high school (.50 to .60), college (.40 to .50) and graduate school (.30 to .40). As a consequence of the increasing drop-out of individuals with lower abilities during secondary school they expect a reduction in variance in g and hence a lower correlation between g and scholastic achievement in higher grade levels. In order to investigate the influence of grade level and to make the results of our analysis comparable to the previous meta-analytic findings, we clustered grade levels into the subgroups Elementary School, Middle School, and High School.

The meta-analysis by Steinkamp and Maehr (1983) did not reveal a significant difference between boys and girls in the correlation between g and scholastic achievement. In the current study, we tested gender as a variable moderating the relationship between g and school grades. We based the analysis on those samples that consisted of either male or female participants.

In order to investigate a potential change in the population correlation between g and scholastic achievement since the previous meta-analyses by Boulanger (1981), Fleming and Malone (1983) and Steinkamp and Maehr (1983), we separated the primary studies into two subgroups including primary studies published before 1983 and those published afterwards.

Section snippets

Inclusion criteria

In this meta-analysis we considered primary studies that fulfilled the following inclusion criteria: (1) The independent variable general mental ability was measured either by standardized intelligence tests or highly comparable tests [e.g., Differential Aptitude Tests (Bennett, Seashore, & Wesman, 1947), Illinois Test of Psycholinguistic Abilities (Kirk, McCarthy, & Kirk, 1974)]. We included primary studies with author-created measures, if it was possible to clearly classify them as

Descriptives

The final data set in our meta-analysis included 162 primary studies published between 1922 and 2014 which provided k = 240 independent samples. The overall sample size was N = 105,185 with sample sizes varying from 15 to 9776. The participants' age at the time their general mental ability was measured was reported for 117 samples. The average age was 13.9 years (SD = 4.0). For a total of 160 samples information on the participant's gender was provided. Thirty-seven samples consisted exclusively of

Discussion

The goal of the present study was to provide a comprehensive and up-to-date meta-analytic examination of the correlation between intelligence and school grades. In doing so, we were additionally interested in evaluating the influence of a set of moderators. The meta-analytic strategy of Hunter and Schmidt (2004) was used, as a correction of statistical artifacts (i.e., sample size, reliability, range restriction) is possible when using this approach. In the following paragraphs the results of

Conclusion

The results of our study clearly show that intelligence has substantial influence on school grades and thus can be regarded as one of the most (if not the most) influential variables in this context. Although intelligence turned out to be a significant predictor on all moderator levels, we were able to identify some scenarios in which even higher validities can be obtained. First of all, the population correlation was highest for tests relying on both verbal and nonverbal materials, indicating

Acknowledgments

We thank two anonymous reviewers for their valuable comments on earlier versions of this article.

References (203)

  • A. Di Fabio et al.

    An in-depth look at scholastic success: Fluid intelligence, personality traits or emotional intelligence?

    Personality and Individual Differences

    (2009)
  • Y.A. Dodonova et al.

    Processing speed and intelligence as predictors of school achievement: Mediation or unique contribution?

    Intelligence

    (2012)
  • A. Fischbach et al.

    Do teacher judgments of student intelligence predict life outcomes?

    Learning and Individual Differences

    (2013)
  • P.A. Freund et al.

    Retest effects in matrix test performance: Differential impact of predictors at different hierarchy levels in an educational setting

    Learning and Individual Differences

    (2011)
  • A. Furnham et al.

    Personality traits and intelligence predict academic school grades

    Learning and Individual Differences

    (2009)
  • J. Gralewski et al.

    Creativity and school grades: A case from Poland

    Thinking Skills and Creativity

    (2012)
  • M. Haugwitz et al.

    Cognitive ability and the instructional efficacy of collaborative concept mapping

    Learning and Individual Differences

    (2010)
  • P.C. Heaven et al.

    When IQ is not everything: Intelligence, personality and academic performance at school

    Personality and Individual Differences

    (2012)
  • M. Hintsanen et al.

    Associations of temperament traits and mathematics grades in adolescents are dependent on the rater but independent of motivation and cognitive ability

    Learning and Individual Differences

    (2012)
  • M. Hofer et al.

    Cognitive ability and personality variables as predictors of school grades and test scores in adolescents

    Learning and Instruction

    (2012)
  • W. Johnson et al.

    Genetic and environmental transactions underlying educational attainment

    Intelligence

    (2009)
  • J. Karbach et al.

    Parental involvement and general cognitive ability as predictors of domain-specific academic achievement in early adolescence

    Learning and Instruction

    (2013)
  • S.B. Kaufman et al.

    Implicit learning as an ability

    Cognition

    (2010)
  • U. Kessels et al.

    Macho-man in school: Toward the role of gender role self-concepts and help seeking in school performance

    Learning and Individual Differences

    (2013)
  • S. Krumm et al.

    Relational integration as a predictor of academic achievement

    Learning and Individual Differences

    (2012)
  • D.C. Adkins

    The efficiency of certain intelligence tests in predicting scholarship scores

    Journal of Educational Psychology

    (1937)
  • ⁎Al-Ghamri, S. J. (2012). A Study in the relationship of mental ability with age, sex, and school achievement of...
  • ⁎Allana, G. (2010). Is academic excellence in school children based on IQ: A review of private schools in Dubai...
  • V. Ames

    Factors related to high-school achievement

    Journal of Educational Psychology

    (1943)
  • W. Arthur et al.

    Conducting meta-analysis using SAS

    (2001)
  • G.S. Aswal

    Intelligence as a correlate of achievement in mathematics across different levels of SES

    Psycholingua

    (2001)
  • G.B. Axelsson

    Mathematical identity in women: The concept, its components and relationship to educative ability, achievement and family support

    International Journal of Lifelong Education

    (2009)
  • K. Barton et al.

    The prediction of school grades from personality and IQ measures

    Personality

    (1971)
  • M. Batey et al.

    Creativity, intelligence, and personality: A critical review of the scattered literature

    Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs

    (2006)
  • C. Beckett et al.

    Institutional deprivation, specific cognitive functions, and scholastic achievement: English and Romanian Adoptee (ERA) study findings

    Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development

    (2010)
  • G.K. Bennett et al.

    Differential aptitude tests

    (1947)
  • L. Blöschl

    BTS, HAWIK, und schulisches Arbeitsverhalten

    Diagnostica

    (1966)
  • ⁎Blue, L. (2009). An investigation into the relationship between cognitive ability, standardized achievement, and...
  • U. Bose

    Scholastic achievement and its correlates

    Psychological Research Journal

    (1982)
  • F.D. Boulanger

    Ability and science learning: A quantitative synthesis

    Journal of Research in Science Teaching

    (1981)
  • F. Boulon-Díaz

    The effects of intelligence, social class, early development and pre-school experience on school achievement of Puerto Rican children

  • ⁎Bowers, J. E. (1966). A study of the relationships among measures of productive thinking intelligence, and ninth-grade...
  • N. Brody

    Intelligence

    (1992)
  • ⁎Brucks, C. A. (1978). Über die Beziehungen zwischen Anspruchsniveau, Intelligenz und Schulleistung (unter...
  • Brunner, M. (2006). Mathematische Schülerleistung: Struktur, Schulform-unterschiede und Validität (unpublished doctoral...
  • R.H. Burgert

    The relation of school marks to intelligence in secondary schools

    Journal of Applied Psychology

    (1937)
  • H.D. Carter

    Improving the prediction of school achievement by the use of the California Study Methods Survey

    Education Administration & Supervision

    (1959)
  • T. Chamorro-Premuzic et al.

    Personality and intellectual competence

    (2005)
  • Cited by (0)

    1

    The authors wish it to be known that, in their opinion, the first two authors should be regarded as joint First Authors.

    References marked with an asterisk are included in the meta-analysis.

    View full text